Humankind
Full Title: Humankind: A Brief History
Author / Editor: Felipe Fernandez-Armesto
Publisher: Oxford University Press, 2004
Review © Metapsychology Vol. 9, No. 13
Reviewer: Mark Welch, Ph.D.
There is little doubt
that Fernández-Armesto likes the big topic. Previous works have been The History of Food, the Millenium and, perhaps with tongue in
cheek modesty, Civilisations. In this
new book he attempts to describe a brief history of what is, and is not, to be
considered human.
He adopts a
historical and anthropological perspective for it is clear that the term
‘humankind’ has by no means meant the same thing to all people at all times. He
ranges between popular culture, high science and religious interpretation in
his rapid and sometimes breathless jaunt around three millennia and almost
every continent (I don’t recall any mention of Antarctica, but I wouldn’t bet
on it). He seems able to extract as much meaning from King Louie, the King of
the Swingers in The Jungle Book,
singing about wanting "to be human too" as the Human Genome Project,
and in this he very successfully meshes serious and popular concerns. He is as
interested in the anthropomorphic interpretation of a visit to the zoo as he is
the added value that exists in just a few cleverly arranged genes that separate
us from other species, or indeed other life forms.
He notes, not without
irony, that in the last two hundred years or so, and very much in the last few
decades, the notion of human values, human rights, human dignity have all grown
in prominence and current debate, while at the same time the ideas of what
actually distinguishes the human from the non-human have grown ever smaller.
Once there was man who held dominion over all the animals of the earth, now it
may be more proper to identify the human race as the third chimpanzee to quote
Jared Diamond. But for a gene or two here or there, things might be very
different.
He charts the
conceptual threat to what it might be to be human as coming from six main
sources. Number one, primatology, which has mounted ever more convincing bodies
of evidence that there is really nothing special or unique about the human at
all, that almost everything that you care to name is, to some degree or
another, performed by other primates; it is all a matter of degree. And so,
rather than distinct categories primatologists would rather have shifting
shades of grey in the primate world and extend the term homo to a much broader population.
Challenge number two
comes from the animal rights movement, many of whom have asked extremely
awkward questions about what makes humans so special. If Shylock had been a
beagle in an animal laboratory rather than a merchant in Venice we might hear
him saying, "If you cut us do we not bleed, if you force us to smoke do we
not cough and get lung cancer, do we not suffer too?" The debate about the
level of sentient beings and consequent natural rights becomes rather difficult
to refute for the modern day liberal. We do not want to go back to the ‘ghost
in the machine’ idea of Descartes, but another adequate answers are hard to
come by. The challenge laid down by contemporary moral philosophers such as
Peter Singer about the value placed on a comatose human in a persistent
vegetative state as opposed to a healthy, sprightly young cow is, at its core,
a challenge to sloppy thinking about humanness.
Challenge number
three comes from paleoanthropology in which Fernández-Armesto argues that
history has been written by the winners and the poor old Neanderthals have been
unjustly maligned like some loser in an ancient war. We humans are not that
unique in whatever characteristic we try to emphasise. Others had big brains
too, walked on two legs, used tools like us; they are just not around to spruik
for themselves.
Challenge number four
comes from a re-evaluation of the accepted norms for the classification of species.
Current taxonomists do not seem quite so certain that the criteria for species
distinction are really that distinct after all. Perhaps many are a matter of
convenience rather than science, and science, as we well know, is not so pure
and unadulterated as we might once have supposed.
Challenge number five
comes from humankind’s own cleverness; something of an irony there. The
increasing interest and recent developments in artificial intelligence, itself
an interesting term as it presumes a non-artificial one (and among the antonyms
that a thesaurus might give to artificial are simple, unflattering and blunt as
well as the more expected natural and true). If a computer can be programmed,
by a human but also by another computer, to perform tasks associated with
humanness, can it actually think? Does Big Blue, chess machine, actually think
creatively or just compute very quickly according to certain algorithms? Can a
computer create? Is creativity the defining condition of humankind? Well, not
if you look at tool using apes, painting elephants or playing dolphins.
The sixth challenge
is from genetics themselves. Genetic engineering has shown how it is possible
to splice genes from one animal to another, and not just animals. So, are
humans really that unique? And even if they are not, are they that special?
Fernández-Armesto
goes on to dismantle in a very effective and elegant manner many of the myths
that have at times held sway, and continue to do so, over our pitiful attempts
to hold on to some uniqueness. He notes the totemic value of animal figures
which embody or signify certain human characteristics or perhaps play a role in
mythologies. Why should this be if it were not for a curious mix of observation
and anthropomorphism. The sly fox, the wise owl, the serpent in the Garden of
Eden are all very familiar, but there is little evidence to suggest that owls
are preternaturally wise and it may be that snakes have just had bad publicity
for a while. But he also shows how humans have an ignoble history of doing this
among themselves. A surprising number of human groups, tribes and remote
peoples, call themselves ‘the people’ or some close variation. Some religious
and ethnic groups call themselves ‘the chosen people’. Clearly, there are
distinctions at work here. Some groups or some characteristics have been
associated with certain values. Racial stereotypes have been evident as long as
they have been recorded, and simply seem to change from time to time, from
place to place, from culture to culture. Thus, when the Roman Empire dominated
the world to be blond and blue eyed was not the ideal, such people were
barbarians, uncouth and incapable of culture. The ancient Egyptians had quite a
different view of black skin than was, or perhaps still is, apparent in Western
culture. The quite extraordinary interest focused on the size and shape of the
genitalia of black people was not all that long ago considered of great
scientific merit. There was also the search for the noble savage, something
more driven by political blind spots than genuine scientific enterprise.
Famously, Lombroso’s taxonomy of criminal types attempted to give scientific
credibility to what is now seen as overt racism; the criminals were
evolutionary throwbacks and the black races more so.
The Twentieth Century
may like to think of itself as a high point in the unbiased scientific
understanding of our world, but it also saw some most ferocious racist dogma
and classification of the worthy and the less so. The Nazi period in Germany or
the Jim Crow laws in the United States are obvious examples, but so are the
sterilization laws and practices across the Western world where until the 1970s
in many countries it was still quite legal to forcibly sterilize those who were
mentally ill or intellectually or physically disabled. Importantly,
Fernández-Armesto reminds us that although the Nazis made eugenics ‘rather
unfashionable for a while" it is now very much back in vogue, and it will
take some sophistry to make a distinction between designer babies and ubermenschen.
The merits of Fernández-Armesto’s account of our
attempts to define ourselves are many. It is full of astonishing glimpses into
histories and cultures and the insights are drawn out with sparkle and wit, and
not a little enjoyment and relish. He loves a clever phrase. But he also has a
slight tendency to be the cleverest boy in school and will sometimes use
obviously difficult or arcane words when something much simpler would suffice.
It may be over-writing to describe the "steatopygous posterior and
hypertrophic ‘apron’" of Saartje Baartmann, the Hottentot Venus, when her
fat bottom would probably do. Nevertheless, there is clearly some very
impressive scholarship in the book, and some deeply provocative and serious
points. He may write as though it is an entertainment, but this book raises
profound questions about not only who we are and who we think we are, but who
we think we are not.
© 2005 Mark Welch
Mark Welch, Ph.D. Assistant Professor in the
Faculty of Nursing at the University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta and
Co-Director of the PAHO/WHO Collaborating Centre for Nursing & Mental
Health.
Categories: General